Skip to main content
Research

Study Highlights Role of Physical, Mental Health in Cognitive Impairment

an older couple walks down the street
Photo credit: Daniel Bernard.

A recent study suggests that preserving physical and mental health helps older adults experiencing cognitive impairment stave off declines in cognitive engagement.

“We found that declines in physical and mental health were associated with more pronounced cognitive disengagement,” says Shevaun Neupert, corresponding author of the study and a professor of psychology at North Carolina State University. “The impact of declines in physical health was particularly pronounced for study participants who had more advanced cognitive impairment to begin with.”

There’s a lot of research showing that cognitive engagement can help older adults maintain cognitive health. However, the vast majority of that work has been done on healthy adults.

“There’s very little work on cognitive engagement in people who are already cognitively impaired, such as people who have been diagnosed with dementia,” Neupert says. “Are they still capable of sustained cognitive engagement? What factors contribute to that engagement?”

To begin addressing those questions, the researchers enlisted 28 study participants. All of the participants were over 60 and had documented cognitive impairment. Participants came to a testing site two times, six months apart. On each visit, researchers collected data on the physical and mental health of the study participants and performed a battery of tests designed to assess cognitive ability. Participants were also connected to a device that tracked blood pressure continuously and then asked to engage in a series of increasingly difficult cognitive tasks. This allowed researchers to track how cognitive engagement changed as the tasks become progressively harder.

Cognitive engagement means taking part in activities that are mentally challenging. Monitoring blood pressure allows the researchers to track how hard study participants are working to accomplish cognitive tasks. Specifically, blood pressure rises as more blood is pumped to the brain when participants work harder at these tasks.

Broadly speaking, the researchers found that if a participant’s cognitive ability, physical health or mental health declined over the course of the six month study period, that participant became less cognitively engaged as the tasks became harder.

“Normally, you’d expect more engagement as the tasks became harder, but we found that some people essentially stopped trying,” says Claire Growney, co-author of the study and a postdoctoral researcher at Washington University in St. Louis.

“The findings highlight the fact that well-being is holistic; physical health, mental health and cognitive function can influence each other,” says Xianghe Zhu, co-author of the paper and a recent Ph.D. graduate of NC State.

“In practical terms, it suggests that it may be particularly important for people to focus on mental and physical well-being during the early stages of cognitive decline,” Growney says. “Or, at the very least, don’t become so focused on addressing cognitive challenges that you ignore physical health, or create anxiety or emotional distress for yourself that leads to mental health problems.”

“Future research will be needed to determine how beneficial it might be for people to take part in cognitively engaging activities once they’ve started experiencing cognitive decline,” Neupert says. “But we already know that there is an element of ‘use it or lose it’ to cognitive function in healthy adults. And while it’s understandable for people to want to avoid tasks that are difficult or challenging, it’s really important to continue challenging ourselves to take part in difficult cognitive activities.”

The paper, “BFF: Bayesian, Fiducial, and Frequentist Analysis of Cognitive Engagement among Cognitively Impaired Older Adults,” is published open access in the journal entropy. The paper was co-authored by Julia Sorensen, a Ph.D. student at NC State; Emily Smith, a postdoctoral researcher at NC State; and Jan Hannig, a professor of statistics and operations research at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

The work was done with support from the National Institute on Aging, under grant R01AG005552-28S1; and from the National Science Foundation, under grants IIS-1633074 and DMS-1916115.

-shipman-

Note to Editors: The study abstract follows.

“BFF: Bayesian, Fiducial, and Frequentist Analysis of Cognitive Engagement among Cognitively Impaired Older Adults”

Authors: Shevaun D. Neupert, Xianghe Zhu, Julia K. Sorensen and Emily L. Smith, North Carolina State University; Claire M. Growney, Washington University in St. Louis; and Jan Hannig, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

Published: April 6, entropy

DOI: 10.3390/e23040428

Abstract: Engagement in cognitively demanding activities is beneficial to preserving cognitive health. Our goal was to demonstrate the utility of frequentist, Bayesian, and fiducial statistical methods for evaluating the robustness of effects in identifying factors that contribute to cognitive engagement for older adults experiencing cognitive decline. We collected a total of 504 observations across two longitudinal waves of data from 28 cognitively impaired older adults. Participants’ systolic blood pressure responsivity, an index of cognitive engagement, was continuously sampled during cognitive testing. Participants reported on physical and mental health challenges and provided hair samples to assess chronic stress at each wave. Using the three statistical paradigms, we compared results from six model testing levels and longitudinal changes in health and stress predicting changes in cognitive engagement. Findings were mostly consistent across the three paradigms, providing additional confidence in determining effects. We extend selective engagement theory to cognitive impairment, noting that health challenges and stress appear to be important moderators. Further, we emphasize the utility of the Bayesian and fiducial paradigms for use with relatively small sample sizes because they are not based on asymptotic distributions. In particular, the fiducial paradigm is a useful tool because it provides more information than p values without the need to specify prior distributions, which may unduly influence the results based on a small sample. We provide the R code used to develop and implement all models.

This post was originally published in NC State News.